Eduvest – Journal of Universal Studies

Volume 3 Number 4, April, 2023

p- ISSN 2775-3735- e-ISSN 2775-3727

 

 

LEXICAL VERB(S) OF HEDGING USED BY THE WHITE HOUSE SECRETARY’S RESPONSES IN PRESS BRIEFING

 

 

Humairotul Husna*, Mohammad Masrukhi

Faculty of Cultural Sciences, Universitas Gadjah Mada Yogyakarta, Indonesia

Email: [email protected]*, [email protected]

 

ABSTRACT

 

The study aims to investigate the lexical verbs of hedging used by two White House secretaries in press briefings, namely Jen Psaki (JP) and Karine Jean Pierre (KJP). The research focuses on the secretary's response in answering the question made by journalists. The corpus consisting of 22 press briefing meetings published from November 2021 to August 2022. The study identifies the most used lexical verbs by the two secretaries through quantitative analysis and qualitative analysis to describe their functions. As a result, the use of hedging verbs is more widely used by secretary Jen Psaki than secretary Karine Jean Pierre. Nevertheless, both used many verbs in the form of think in their responses. The findings reveal the uncertainty and reduce the secretary’s commitment. Besides, the function of hedging is to protect secretaries from forward-looking statements and seek protection against overstatement.

 

KEYWORDS

Hedging verb; responses; press briefing

 

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International

 

 

INTRODUCTION

The responses made by the White House secretary to answer the question seem to have their style in every spoken speech. The word seems, think, probably, would, could, might are often uttered in spoken language, especially in the press briefing. These devices are used for specific purposes such as ‘vague,' 'evade,' 'fuzzy,' and ‘uncertainty’. The words whose meaning shows fuzziness are first created by Lakoff (1973). Following Lakoff (1973), it is known as ‘hedging’ forms in Linguistics. Biber et al. (1999) have argued that hedging demonstrates linguistic expression that decreases the consequence of intentional statements expressing possibilities, uncertainty, and inaccuracy. It is usually used to indicate tentative and uncertain an assertion that can be interpreted differently depending on the conversation's content and who employs the hedges.

Moreover, hedging belongs an essential strategy used in communication. It follows Hyland (1998) that hedging is used to soften statements' accuracy, claim the precision, and show doubts. In academic writing, hedging is used by the author to reduce the strength of what has been written (Rabab’ah & Abu Rumman, 2015). According to Biber et al. (1999), hedging is displayed in linguistic expression expressing uncertainty, possibilities, and inaccuracy, showing the consequent reduction of intentional statements. According to Hyland (1998), Hedging is weakening statements' accuracy, claiming accuracy, and expressions of doubt. Itani (1998) claimed that the word probably and suppose is considered hedging devices.

Hedging is an essential strategy employed in political Press Briefing (Schubert, 2014; Hassan, 2016; Fu & Wang, 2022; Yang & Li, 2022). Such as, the White House secretary gave transparent responses to answer various journalists' questions about government information. Addai & Gurji (2021) revealed that there had been relevant research on equivocation, vagueness, and uncertainty, especially in politicians' speeches. According to McLaren-Hankin (2008), press releases aim to communicate with the public to inform them about their work. It is related to the past and present and the case for the future involving the community. Press briefings involve prediction, which frequently reveals a forward-looking statement. What the secretary conveyed in the press briefing is employing the hedging displayed in the responses. The secretary should employ hedging in her utterances primarily due to the occupational responsibility to be alert to the trouble of her speech in a press briefing (Resche, 2004). That is the importance of using hedging against the consequences that can occur. According to Dornbusch et al. (1988), public reactions depend mainly on their interpretation of policy decisions. Therefore, the secretary is fully aware that she has many opportunities for public appearance when conveying inaccurate information will take a loss of credibility. Brown & Levinson (1987) stated that hedging is "a particle, word or phrase that modify the degree of membership of a predicate or a noun phrase in a set; it says of that membership that it is partial or true only in certain respects, or that it is more true and complete than perhaps might be expected".

Tang (2013) pointed out that the classification of hedging usage is due to the perspective of the study. It also forms with an even smaller scope as in words and phrases. According to Liu (2020), Many words fulfill the standard of hedging usage that can be classified as adverbs, modal verbs, adjectives, and impersonal phrases. Hedging can appear in various linguistic forms, for instance, lexical, grammatical, and syntactical (Hardjanto, 2016).

Many researchers have conducted hedges. Some researchers analyzed the hedges such as in the article (Hardjanto, 2016; Mur-Dueñas, 2021; Hu & Cao, 2011; Budiarti, 2019; Triyoko, 2021), written text (Salager-Meyer, 1994), text message (Walker et al., 2011), conversation (Anindyawati, 2004; Wang, 2010; Engström, 2018), gender (Namaziandost & Shafiee, 2018; Mohajer & Jan 2015; Li & Li, 2020), book reviews (Itakura, 2013), broadcast (Yang & Yap, 2015), and discourse (Pisanski Peterlin & Zlatnar Moe, 2016; Plappert, 2019).

Although the word hedging tool has been widely used in many previous kinds of research, both written and spoken, the word hedging may appear in various forms and variations. It is considering Brown & Levinson (1987) that the potential for hedging disclosure is not semantically limited in its number of forms. It can be found in any situation, and hedging devices are widely used in many cases with different words, phrases, or clauses. The present study will investigate the use of the lexical verb(s) in the secretary's response in obtaining information related to public policy, and what the government is doing will be conveyed transparently and also identify the function of using hedging. Moreover, the study aims to investigate the lexical verbs of hedging used by two White House secretaries in press briefings, namely Jen Psaki (JP) and Karine Jean Pierre (KJP).

 

Epistemic modality

According to Perkins (1983), Epistemic means a lack of knowledge. Coates (1983) defined epistemic as “the speaker’s assumptions, or assessment of possibilities, and, in most cases, it indicates the speaker’s confidence or lack of confidence in the truth of the proposition expressed”. Vold (2006) defined epistemic modality as a linguistic expression that expresses possibility, for example, perhaps. Epistemic modality is characterized by how people rely on the truth of a proposition. Depraetere & Reed (2008) argued that the use of epistemic modality does not show factual information or indicate the possible truth of a proposition.

On the other hand, Le Querler (1996) claimed that epistemic modality is the way the total uncertainty toward absolute certainty. It can be concluded that epistemic modality is a speaker's strategy to express doubts about some information based on specific knowledge and considerations or propositions that can be judged both true or false. Palmer (2001) formulated the examples of the epistemic sentence using three modal verbs:

(1)  John may be in his office.

(2)  John must be in his office.

(3)  John will be in his office.

The three sentences above have different propositional judgments. Sentence (1) describes the speaker’s uncertainty about John’s presence in the office. The second sentence shows that the speaker makes a judgment based on the evidence (e.g., that the office light is on or hearing John’s voice from outside), and the third sentence means the speaker generally knows about John (from a habit that John often does). Therefore, from those explanations, it can be concluded that the proposition presented is a possibility based on specific considerations.

Hedging’s Classification

Some categories of hedging highlighted the concept of Hyland (1998), Namasaraev (1997), and Varttala (1999) can be formulated as follows:

 

 

 

 

 

Table 1. Categories of Hedging

Hedging forms

Types

Parts

Examples

Lexical

Verb

Modal

Can, may, must, shall, will

Lexical

Claim, think, suggest, assert

Adverb

Frequency

Commonly, frequently, often, typically

Degree

Highly, strongly, significantly.

Probability

Possibly, presumably, seemingly.

Approximative

About, almost, around, some, approximately.

Adjective

Frequency

Common, frequent, general

Degree

Central, large, significant

Probability

Likely, perhaps, possible.

Approximative

Approximate, close

Noun

Tentative cognitive

Estimate, assessment, perception

Nonfactive assertive

Argument, prediction, indication

Tentative likelihood

Opportunity, possibility, potential

Grammatical

Impersonal construction

It- clause

It is likely…, it was possible that…it is suggested that…

Personification

The results indicate that…

The data show…

Interrogative construction

Question

 

Conditional construction

 

Real

 

If clause

Unreal

 

Based on the table above, the verb hedging consists of a modal auxiliary and lexical verb. The word may, will, shall, and must belong to modal auxiliary verbs, while be, have, and do are non-modal auxiliary verbs. Besides, the word claim, think, and suggest include a lexical verb, while non-modal lexical verbs are drink, come, and eat (Budiarti, 2019). The primary device in these verb categories, often identified as hedging, is modal verbs related to the present and the past condition (Huddleston & Pullum, 2005). Besides, lexical verbs are also indicated as hedging devices which will be the focus of study in this research. By so doing, this study used the concept of Palmer (2001), which divided the lexical verb of hedging into four categories.

 

Table 2. Lexical Verbs Categories

Categories

Examples

Explanation

Speculative

Think, argue, believe, suggest

State a claim based on his own subjective opinion

Deductive

Assume, conclude, interpret, presume

State a conclusion based on the statements he/ she made earlier

Quotative

"Peter, the President believes in the rule of law. The President said that he believes in the independence of the Department of Justice".

Quote someone's statement.

Sensorial

Feel, see, look, appear

Involve the use of the senses.

 

Press briefing

The press briefing is a press conference held by the President's secretary. The President's secretary holds the press conference, and the journalists attend. The event is a question-and-answer interaction held by journalists with the secretary of the white house about the topic related to issues that existed in society. A government organization presents press briefings to convey the information to the public through the media. The press is led by a secretary to choose the reporters to ask questions related to the topic. It is essential to know because a press briefing represents the President's policy and daily issues in the nation, even related to the world. The secretary conveys the brief information as a president's representative to provide the information transparently, give the responses and present the information that is still a question for the public to be delivered through the press briefing. It became an extensive information and referral source to journalists in publishing an issue to become public consumption. Hedging can be an essential communication strategy the secretary employs in giving a response. The use of hedging will likely differ based on the speaker's wishes. The speakers can choose what language they want to convey. This study is interested in identifying the use of hedging by the White House secretaries, namely Jen Psaki and Karine Jean-Pierre. Even though they both served as secretaries of the White House, the use of hedging may seem different. The researcher investigates the hedges used by the secretary in conveying crucial information related to the news within the government. The previous research was used as a reference to investigate the hedges in the secretary's responses.

 

 

RESEARCH METHOD

The study analyzes the responses given by the white house secretary. The responses aim to answer the questions asked by journalists attending the press briefing. The data is in the form of sentences containing the dispreferred responses, giving information, and straightening the argument. The response will be obtained from the secretary who once served as secretary of the White House, namely Jen Psaki, who served as secretary from January 2021 to May 2022 and then continued by Karine Jean Pierre until now. Purposive sampling will use in the research. It suggests that the data will be applied from November 2021 to December 2022. Besides, the data merely 16 focussed on the responses of the secretaries. The data is acquired from the official website of the White House, which can be seen and downloaded at https://www.whitehouse.gov/briefing-room/.

The video of the press briefing by the White House Press Secretary also can be seen and downloaded on the official White House YouTube account. The research employs a quantitative method to find the frequency and distribution of hedging forms. At the same time, to find the function of the hedging, use a descriptive qualitative method. The data collection steps to analyze the hedges in press briefing are formulated as follows: Collecting the conversation data in press briefing transcription of the Press Briefing, separating the conversation into two parts (questions and responses), Thoroughly reading the responses, and classifying the data into hedging (Classify the data precisely in words, sentences, or clauses). After downloading the responses, the document format from the transcript, which initially had the extension "word," was converted to plaintext. This conversion is done considering the software used in this study is AntConc 3.5.9 (Anthony, 2020), which can only process the files with that extension.

Furthermore, the data is processed through a keyword search method in the Corpus by entering one by one word that is classified as hedging. At this stage, keywords that have appeared will be filtered because not necessarily all of them appear to include hedging. After acquiring the data, the researcher identifies and analyzes the hedging verbs found in a press briefing by White House Press Secretary: Jen Psaki and Karine Jean Pierre based on the theory of Palmer (2001) In the end, the researcher contributes conclusions.

 

RESULT AND DISCUSSION

In this section, the authors present an analysis concerning hedging used by secretaries in their responses. First, it displays the frequency of forms of hedging used according to Palmer (2001), as well as a comparison both of the frequencies of the two white house secretaries, Jen Psaki (JP) and Karine Jean Pierre (KJP). In addition to modal auxiliary verbs, lexical verbs also play an essential position in expressing hedging. The hedging verb in this study will be limited to several lexical verbs often found in press briefings, such as think, guess, seem(s), argue, believe, conclude, feel, suggest, predict, imply, expect, and suppose. Below is two White House secretaries in their responses: the frequency of using hedging.

 

Table 3. Frequency of Hedging Verbs by Two White House Secretaries

Secretary

Number of Words

Frequency

Percentage

JP

112.407

503

59%

KJP

113.354

356

41%

Total

225.761

859

100%

 

Referring to the table above is generally the use of hedging in the form of lexical verbs by two press briefing secretaries at the white house. The table above shows that the use of hedging in the form of lexical verbs is more widely used in the response of secretary Jen Psaki (59%) than in secretary Karine Jean Pierre (41%). While, Surprisingly, the number of words that appear in the response tends to be more than the number in Secretary Jean Pierre's response. Besides, in this case, there is a level of awareness of the two secretaries as spokespersons for the government of the importance of using hedging, especially in the form of lexical verbs in discussing political issues and government policies in conveying them to the public. In order to be more detailed, the following will show one by one the use of hedging in the form of lexical verbs used by the two press secretaries.

 

 

 

 

 

Table 4. The Form and Frequency of the use of Lexical Verbs Used by Secretary Jen Psaki

Verb of hedging

JP

Frequency

Percentage

Think

296

0.59

Expect

74

0.15

Believe 

45

0.09

Feel

37

0.07

Guess

14

0.03

Predict

13

0.03

Suggest

8

0.02

Seem(s)

7

0.01

Conclude

4

0.01

Argue

3

0.01

Imply 

1

0.0

Suppose 

1

0.0

Total

503

1.00

 

Table (4) demonstrates that the word "think” is the most frequently used by Jean Psaki in her responses, with a frequency of 0.59. The word “think" is often used in spoken language, especially in a press briefing (Fu & Wang, 2022). While the word “expect” is the second most common word after the word “think” in Secretary Jen Psaki's response (0.15). There are significant differences in the use of think and expect. Then, the third order of the most used lexical verbs is “believe," with a frequency of 0.09 and followed by the word feel (0.07), guess and predict with the same frequency (0.03), the use of word suggest (0.02), seem, conclude and argue (0.01). The minor lexical verb used in Secretary Jen Psaki's response is the word imply and suppose with frequency (0,0).

 

Table 5. The form and frequency of the use of lexical verbs used by secretary Karine Jean Pierre

Verb of hedging

KJP

Frequency

Percentage

Think

224

0.63

Believe

81

0.23

Feel

19

0.05

Expect

15

0.04

Argue

12

0.03

Conclude

2

0.01

Predict

1

0.00

Guess

1

0.00

Seem(s)

1

0.00

Suggest

0

0.00

Imply

0

0.00

Suppose

0

0.00

Total

356

1.00

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

In table (5), the word think is the most frequently used in secretary Karine Jean Pierre’s response, with a frequency of 0.63. It is not much different from the secretary Jen Psaki where the most widely used word is “think”. It indicates that the use of think in press secretary is evenly used. Unlike Jen Psaki, the second most used lexical verb by secretary Karine Jean Pierre is the word believe with a frequency (of 0.23). The word feel is much different from the word believe, which ranks third with a frequency of 0.05. Then, followed by the use of the word expect (0.04), argue (0.03), and conclude (0.01). Surprisingly, if in the response of secretary Jen Psaki there is the use of the words imply, suggest, and suppose, then there is a difference in the response of secretary Karine Jean Pierre who does not use these words in the press response at all. It can be concluded that the use of hedging for the two secretaries has a significant difference. It can be seen that Karine Jean Pierre's secretary tends to use less hedging in her response than Jen Psaki's secretary.

Speculative lexical verbs

Basically, speculative lexical verbs are used in the form of performative verbs (argue, propose, suggest). Besides that, it can also be realized in the form of cognitive verbs such as believe and think. Speculative lexical verbs are general clauses that use personal pronouns (i) and (we) as subjects. As follows:

I think, in President Biden's heart, he is a local elected official still, and he gets into the weeds of what they're experiencing. (JP02)

I think he mentioned this in his - in his statement yesterday, he

has spoken to Senator Schumer. (KJP17)

I believe it's a commission at the Department of Defense. It's certainly an

initiative the President of the United States supports, but I would send you to the Department of Defense for more specifics about the timeline and membership. (JP12)

And so, again, the President se- - believes that it is his job to level with the American people about threats he sees to our nation and our values. And that's what you heard from him last night. And we believe it did - it indeed resonated. (KJP05)

The use and choice of the think verb in a secretary's response like the examples above are generally included an opinion based on knowledge of the matter concerned. This can be said to be a subjective or tentative interpretation and still conveys the truth of the proposition. The use of the lexical verbs listed above is considered a hedging device since there is a subjective cognitive notion of press secretaries. In another sense, the proposition submitted cannot be ascertained by the secretary but is suspected to be true because it is not the result of objective calculations (Varttala, 2001). It is also claimed by Hyland (1998) that the use of cognitive lexical verbs is a presumption meaning of the previous proposition.

 

Deductive lexical verbs

Deductive lexical verbs are preceded by subject (i) and (we), as well as speculative lexical verbs. Explicitly, this proposition is the result of the speaker's conclusions.

I suppose - about the meeting this weekend kind of got out there, and obviously Brian confirmed it. There are a lot of meetings happening at one time with a lot of different officials. (JP03)

The deductive verbs above show inferential reasoning on previous opinions or knowledge (Budiarti, 2019). Vass (2017) argued that deductive verbs are used on a set of known facts in making conclusions.

 

Quotative lexical verbs

Suppose some speculative lexical verbs are marked with personal pronouns (i) and (we). In that case, the quotative lexical verbs generally have a third-person subject which is realized by quoting a particular name or object. Like the two types of lexical verbs above, the verbs used most often in the responses of the two white house press secretaries are the verbs think and believe, especially using personal pronouns I and we. Here are the examples below:

Well, the President believes that the Senate has the constitutional duty to proceed as they see fit on holding the former President accountable. He spent 36 years in the Senate; he's no longer there. (JP05)

But, you know, the President thinks that there is an extremist threat to our democracy. (KJP07)

In the press response of the white house secretary, many forms of quoted lexical verbs are found in the form of believe and think. It is due to his authority over work as a government spokesperson or President. The secretary conveys what the President does and responds to a particular policy. In this way, they directly provide propositions based on people who are very influential to the public and make the public believe in certain information.

 

Sensorial lexical verbs

The use of lexical verbs in this type generally involves using the senses. The quotative lexical verbs included include the words see, feel and look. The verb feel is most used in the responses of the two white house secretaries.

So, don't want to - you know, we feel like it's inappropriate to talk about hypotheticals here. (JP02)

We feel that we have the time - some time to make that happen. And so, that is - in order to continue our progress and build on our progress, we're going to continue to fight for that funding. (KJP05)

In this case, quotative lexical verbs reveal the secretary's attitude, inviting the public to agree with what he is saying. Based on the overall presentation of the data above, the use of lexical verbs by the two secretary's responses is the preference of each secretary in selecting lexical verbs to use according to the function they want to use according to the context. In short, the use of lexical verbs by the two secretaries is not much different in their usage pattern. The difference appears in the comparison of the number of uses of lexical verbs.

As previously mentioned, one of the hedging functions is to reduce the speaker's commitment. This case supports McLaren-Hankin's (2008) research that hedging functions to protect secretaries from forward-looking statements. It is what Hyland (1998) has stated that forward-looking statements involve uncertainty. Hyland (ibid) has argued that hedging has a sign of uncertainty in making assertions and seeking protection against overstatement. The secretaries can never be 100 percent sure of their arguments. To overcome this, secretaries often involve the President in their statements. It can be seen in the example below:

The President put an increase in the minimum wage in the initial package because he thinks it's important for American men and women, who are - have a full-time job, working hard, to have a decent wage, and he thinks it's long past time to raise the minimum wage. But we'll let the process see itself through, and I'm not going to negotiate what he'd be open to and not from the podium. (JP18)

Indeed, as well as showing caution in making statements to avoid uncertain knowledge, the use of hedging in this case also involves predictions, expectations, and beliefs. It is a positive attitude in conducting press briefings to outline performance in the future, the policies to be taken, and expectations regarding the development of something going forward. Besides, In addition, the secretary's moral attitude in giving arguments with several sentences that use the pronoun (we) as a form of expression for the spokesperson not to express his point of view directly (Zhou, 2020). Furthermore, in a press conference, as shown in this study, the spokesperson does not have to raise his voice or be agitated and openly confront journalists. However, they have a strategy to evade that by preventing journalists from continuing with their unpleasant questions. It is generally because the secretary wants to have a positive image of government performance and strengthen government authority.

 

 

CONCLUSION

This study examines hedging, specifically in the lexical verbs used by two white house secretaries in a press briefing, by identifying the most used lexical verbs by the two secretaries through quantitative analysis and qualitative analysis to describe the accompanying functions. The findings reveal that the use of personal opinion (think) is primarily used by the two secretaries than other lexical verbs. In addition, it is common to involve the President in expressing arguments in press briefings. It is done to form a positive image and gain public belief. Finally, this study opens broader insights into evaluating the response given by the white house secretary. However, this study has a limitation, namely due to the limited research on lexical verbs. This research still needs to determine hedging at different levels for a broader comparison.

 

REFERENCES

Addai, S. M., & Gurji, R. J. (2021). Hedging In Trump Speeches About Covid-19ة, June, 51. https://doi.org/10.33811/1847-005-003-003

Anindyawati, M. (2004). Hedges Usage in Mixed-Gender Conversation among English Department Students Airlangga University. 2002, 21–27.

Biber, D., Leech, G., & Johansson, S. (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow: Pearson Education Limited.

Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage (Studies in Interactional Sociolinguistics 4). In TESOL Quarterly (Vol. 22, Issue 4). https://doi.org/10.2307/3587263

Budiarti, D. (2019). Verba Leksikal Pengungkap Pembentengan dalam Artikel Penelitian Berbahasa Inggris oleh Penutur Jati dan Nonjati. Universitas Gadjah Mada.

Coates, J. (1983). The Sematics of Modal Auxiliaries (pp. 27–64).

Depraetere, I., & Reed, S. (2008). Mood and Modality in English. The Handbook of English Linguistics, 269–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470753002.ch12

Dornbusch, R., Fisher, S., & Startz, R. (1988). Macroeconomics.

Engström, A. (2018). I ’ m sure women use more hedges , I think A study comparing male and female usage of hedges.

Fu, R., & Wang, K. (2022). Hedging in interpreted and spontaneous speeches: A comparative study of Chinese and American political press briefings. Text and Talk, 42(2), 153–175. https://doi.org/10.1515/text-2019-0290

Hardjanto, T. D. (2016). Pembentengan dalam Artikel Penelitian Ilmiah dalam Bahasa Inggris. Disertasi Doktor Fakultas Ilmu Budaya Universitas Gadjah Mada.

Hassan, M. A. H. H. (2016). Hedging In press Briefing. 70, 605–626.

Hu, G., & Cao, F. (2011). Hedging and boosting in abstracts of applied linguistics articles: A comparative study of English- and Chinese-medium journals. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(11), 2795–2809. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2011.04.007

Huddleston, R., & Pullum, G. K. (2005). A Student’s Introduction to English Grammar. Cambridge University.

Hyland, K. (1998). Persuasion and context: The pragmatics of academic metadiscourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 30(4), 437–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0378-2166(98)00009-5

Itakura, H. (2013). Hedging praise in English and Japanese book reviews. Journal of Pragmatics, 45(1), 131–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.11.003

Itani, R. (1998). Semantics and Pragmatics of Hedges in English and Japanese. Pragmatics, 8(4), 584–585.

Lakoff, G. (1973). Hedges: A Study in Meaning Criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts. Philosophical Logic, 15(4), 250–260. https://doi.org/10.11436/mssj.15.250

Le Querler, N. (1996). Typolofie des modalités. Presses universitaires de Caen.

Li, X., & Li, F. (2020). Gender Difference in Hedging: A Corpus-Based Study to TED Talks about Emotion. Creative Education, 11(10), 2106–2115. https://doi.org/10.4236/ce.2020.1110153

Liu, J. (2020). A pragmatic analysis of hedges from the perspective of politeness principle. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 10(12), 1614–1619. https://doi.org/10.17507/tpls.1012.15

McLaren-Hankin, Y. (2008). “We expect to report on significant progress in our product pipeline in the coming year”: Hedging forward-looking statements in corporate press releases. Discourse Studies, 10(5), 635–654. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445608094216

Mohajer, L., & Jan, J. M. (2015). Preserving Face and the Use of Hedges in Masculine World of Men. In Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences (Vol. 208, Issue Icllic 2014). Elsevier B.V. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.11.176

Mur-Dueñas, P. (2021). There may be differences: Analysing the use of hedges in English and Spanish research articles. Lingua, 260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2021.103131

Namasaraev, V. (1997). Hedging in Russian Academic Writing in Sociological Text. Publishing Company.

Namaziandost, E., & Shafiee, S. (2018). Gender Differences in the Use of Lexical Hedges in Academic Spoken Language among Iranian EFL Learners: A Comparative Study. International Journal of Research in English Education, 3(4), 63–80. https://doi.org/10.29252/ijree.3.4.63

Palmer, F. R. (2001). Mood and Modality. Cambridge University Press.

Perkins, M. R. (1983). Modal Expression in English (Norwood (ed.)). Ablex Publishing Corporation.

Pisanski, P. A., & Zlatnar Moe, M. (2016). Translating hedging devices in news discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 102, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2016.06.009

Plappert, G. (2019). Not hedging but implying: Identifying epistemic implicature through a corpus-driven approach to scientific discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 139, 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2018.09.001

Rabab’ah, G., & Abu Rumman, R. (2015). Hedging in Political Discourse: Evidence from the Speeches of King Abdullah II of Jordan. Prague Journal of English Studies, 4(1), 157–185. https://doi.org/10.1515/pjes-2015-0009

Resche, C. (2004). Investigating “Greenspanese”: From hedging to “fuzzy transparency.” Discourse and Society, 15(6), 723–744. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926504046502

Salager-Meyer, F. (1994). Hedges and textual communicative function in medical English written discourse. English for Specific Purposes, 13(2), 149–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/0889-4906(94)90013-2

Schubert, C. (2014). I haven’t spoken to him about it. Language and Dialogue, 4(1), 58–75. https://doi.org/10.1075/ld.4.1.04sch

Tang, J. (2013). Pragmatic functions of hedges and politeness principles. International Journal of Applied Linguistics and English Literature, 2(4), 155–160. https://doi.org/10.7575/aiac.ijalel.v.2n.4p.155

Triyoko, H. (2021). Kategori, Makna, dan Fungsi Piranti Pembentengan dalam Artikel Ilmiah Berbahasa Indonesia. Universitas Gadjah Mada.

Varttala, T. (1998). Remarks on the Communicative Functions of Hedging in Popular Scientific and Specialist Research Articles on Medicine. English for Specific Purposes, 18(2), 177–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0889-4906(98)00007-6

Vass, H. (2017). Lexical Verb Hedging in Legal Discourse: The Case of Law Journal Articles and Supreme Court Majority and Dissenting Opinions. English for Specific Purpose, 20, 83–102.

Vold, E. T. (2006). Epistemic modality markers in research articles: A cross-linguistic and cross-disciplinary study. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 16(1), 61–87. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1473-4192.2006.00106.x

Walker, T., Drew, P., & Local, J. (2011). Responding indirectly. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(9), 2434–2451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2011.02.012

Wang, Y. (2010). Analyzing Hedges in Verbal Communication: An Adaptation-Based Approach. English Language Teaching, 3(3), 120–124. https://doi.org/10.5539/elt.v3n3p120

Yang, Y., & Yap, F. H. (2015). “I am sure but I hedge”: Fear expression kǒngpà as an interactive rhetorical strategy in Mandarin broadcast talk. Journal of Pragmatics, 83, 41–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2015.04.013

Yang, Z., & Li, L. (2022). No straight talk here: A multi-level analysis of hedging strategies employed by the Fed Chair in press conferences. Journal of Pragmatics, 188, 141–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2021.12.004

Zhou, L. (2020). Moral stance taking as a device of covert aggression in Chinese political language use. Discourse, Context and Media, 36, 100415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcm.2020.100415